《電大《大學(xué)英語B》統(tǒng)考題庫試卷小抄》由會員分享,可在線閱讀,更多相關(guān)《電大《大學(xué)英語B》統(tǒng)考題庫試卷小抄(41頁珍藏版)》請在裝配圖網(wǎng)上搜索。
1、
《大學(xué)英語B》統(tǒng)考題庫試卷
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
一:交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
2、
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
交際英語
二:閱讀理解
二:閱讀理解
3、
二:閱讀理解
二:閱讀理解
二:閱讀理解
閱讀理解
閱讀理解
閱讀理解
4、
三:詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
5、
三:詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
三:詞匯與語法
四:完形填空:
21、 22、 23、24、
6、
25、 26、 27、 28、
29、30、
四:完形填空:
21、 22、 23、 24、
25、 26、 27、 28、
29、 30、
四:完形填空:
21、 22、 23、 24、
25、 26、 27、 28、
29、 30、
四:完形填空:
21、 22、 23、 24、
25、 26、 27、 28、
29、30、
四:完形填空:
四:完形填空:
21、 22、
7、23、 24、
25、 26、 27、 28、
29、 30、
四:完形填空:
21、 22、 23、 24、
25、 26、 27、 28、
29、 30、
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29
30
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29 30
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29
30
完型填空
21 22 23 24
25 26 27 28 29
8、30
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29 30
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29 30
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29 30
完型填空
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29 30
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29 30
完型填空
21 22 23 24 25
26 27 28 29 30
21. 22. 23
9、. 24.
25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30.
21. 22. 23.
24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30.
21. 22. 23. 24.
25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30.
21. 21. 23. 24. 25. 26.
27. 28. 29.
30.
21. 22.
10、 23. 24. 25. 26.
27. 28. 29. 30.
21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26.
27. 28. 29.
30.
21. 22. 23. 24.
25. 26. 27. 28.
29. 30.
Why do we like music? Like most good questions, this one works on many levels. We hav
11、e answers on some levels, but not all.
We like music because it makes us feel good. Why does it make us feel good? In 2001, neuroscientists Anne Blood and Robert Zatorre at McGill University in Montreal provided an answer. Using magnetic resonance imaging they showed that people listening to pleasu
12、rable music had activated brain regions called the limbic and paralimbic areas, which are connected to euphoric reward responses, like those we experience from sex, good food and addictive drugs. Those rewards come from a gush of a neurotransmitter called dopamine. As DJ Lee Haslam told us, music is
13、 the drug.
But why? It’s easy enough to understand why sex and food are rewarded with a dopamine rush: this makes us want more, and so contributes to our survival and propagation. (Some drugs subvert that survival instinct by stimulating dopamine release on false pretences.) But why would a sequenc
14、e of sounds with no obvious survival value do the same thing?
The truth is no one knows. However, we now have many clues to why music provokes intense emotions. The current favourite theory among scientists who study the cognition of music – how we process it mentally – dates back to 1956, when the
15、 philosopher and composer Leonard Meyer suggested that emotion in music is all about what we expect, and whether or not we get it. Meyer drew on earlier psychological theories of emotion, which proposed that it arises when we’re unable to satisfy some desire. That, as you might imagine, creates frus
16、tration or anger – but if we then find what we’re looking for, be it love or a cigarette, the payoff is all the sweeter.
This, Meyer argued, is what music does too. It sets up sonic patterns and regularities that tempt us to make unconscious predictions about what’s coming next. If we’re right, the
17、 brain gives itself a little reward – as we’d now see it, a surge of dopamine. The constant dance between expectation and outcome thus enlivens the brain with a pleasurable play of emotions.
Why should we care, though, whether our musical expectations are right or not? It’s not as if our life depen
18、ded on them. Ah, says musicologist David Huron of Ohio State University, but perhaps once it did. Making predictions about our environment – interpreting what we see and hear, say, on the basis of only partial information – could once have been essential to our survival, and indeed still often is, f
19、or example when crossing the road. And involving the emotions in these anticipations could have been a smart idea. On the African savannah, our ancestors did not have the luxury of mulling over whether that screech was made by a harmless monkey or a predatory lion. By bypassing the “l(fā)ogical brain” a
20、nd taking a shortcut to the primitive limbic circuits that control our emotions, the mental processing of sound could prompt a rush of adrenalin – a gut reaction – that prepares us to get out of there anyway.
We all know that music has this direct line to the emotions: who hasn’t been embarrassed b
21、y the tears that well up as the strings swell in a sentimental film, even while the logical brain protests that this is just cynical manipulation? We can’t turn off this anticipatory instinct, nor its link to the emotions – even when we know that there’s nothing life-threatening in a Mozart sonata. “Nature’s tendency to overreact provides a golden opportunity for musicians”, says Huron. “Composers can fashion passages that manage to provoke remarkably strong emotions using the most innocuous stimuli imaginable.”